COA At-a-Glance
Evidence of cognitive interviewing of draft instrument in target patient population
Evidence of internal consistency
Evidence of test-retest or inter-rater reliability
Evidence of concurrent validity
Evidence of known-groups validity
Evidence of ability to detect change over time
Evidence of responder thresholds
Inclusion of the COA in product labelling
- Overview
- Content Validity
- Reliability
- Validity
- Ability to Detect Change
- Responder Thresholds
- Reference(s) of development / validation
- Other references
- Inclusion of the COA in product labelling
- Existence of Scoring / Interpretation / User Manual
- Original language and translations
- References of translations
- Authors and contact information
- Condition of use: copyright
- Website
- Review copy
Overview
Instrument Name: MacArthur-Bates Communication Development Inventories-Words and Gestures
Abbreviation: MB-CDI-WG
Points for Consideration:
Appropriate for individuals with limited spoken communication. Based on caregiver report, so it's helpful to walk parents through what is being asked. Good option for individuals with significant communication delays who may struggle with in-clinic testing. Recommend using the latest editions.
Description of Tool:
The MacArthur-Bates Communication Development Inventories-Words and Gestures is an ObsRO developed to measure language growth in children aged 8 to 16 months (All, Language Development Disorders).
Other Related Tools (if applicable):
MacArthur-Bates Communication Development Inventories-Words and Sentences
Minimum Qualification Required by COA Administrator: No degree requirement
Comment:
The original version of the inventories was developed in 1975 by Bates, Camaioni, & Volterra, but the inventories have much changed since then
The MB-CDI is divided into two parts. The CDI/Words and Gestures (CDI-WG) is designed for children 8 to 16 months old (also referred to as CDI Infant form, and the CDI/Words and Sentences (CDI-WS) is designed for children 16 to 30 months old (also referred to as the CDI toddler form).
Short versions of the MB-CDI also exist as well as the MB-CDI III which includes an extension for three year olds
Year: 1993
Objective of Development:
To measure language growth in children with and without disabilities
Population of Development: Age range (therapeutic indication):
8 to 16 months (All, Language Development Disorders)
Pediatric Population(s) in which COA has been used:
MB-CDI:
Mental Disorders; Female Urogenital Diseases and Pregnancy Complications; Otorhinolaryngologic Diseases; Nervous System Diseases; Pathological Conditions, Signs and Symptoms; Respiratory Tract Diseases; Infections; Congenital, Hereditary, and Neonatal Diseases and Abnormalities; Cardiovascular Diseases; Musculoskeletal Diseases; Stomatognathic Diseases; Chemically-Induced Disorders
COA type:
Number of Items 493
Mode of Administration:
Data Collection Mode:
Time for Completion: None identified
Response Scales: Dichotomous: Yes / No; Understand / Understand and says 3-point Likert/Likert-type Scale ranging from "Not yet" to "Often" Others: Checklist; Open-ended question
Summary of Scoring:
Available Scores:
Global score
Score by domains
Weighting:
No
Score Direction:
Higher score = Better communicative skills
Content Validity
Evidence of Literature Review: Yes
Evidence of Instrument Review: Yes
Evidence of Clinical or Expert Input: Yes
Evidence of concept elicitation in target patient population: Yes
Evidence of a Saturation Grid: None identified
Evidence for Selection of Data Collection Method: Yes
Recall/Observation Period:
Evidence for Selection of Reponse Options: None identified
Evidence of cognitive interviewing of draft instrument in target patient population: Yes
Evidence of Preliminary Scoring of Items and Domains: Yes
Evidence related to respondent and administrator burden: None identified
Evidence of a Conceptual Framework: None identified
Evidence of an item-tracking matrix: None identified
Evidence related to item selection: Yes
Evidence of re-testing the final version: Yes
Reliability
Internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha): Yes
Evidence of internal consistency:
Test-retest Reliability (ICC):
Fenson L (1994)
- Pearson's correlation coefficient: p<0.01 for all results
Vocabulary comprehension: 0.87
Vocabulary production: 0.95
Action and gesture: 0.86
- Was a definition of stability applied to identify stable patients: No
- Time frame or interval between the two administrations: Mean time frame of 1.35 months
- Population/Disease: Children aged from 8 to 16 months ; n= 137
Evidence of test-retest or inter-rater reliability: Yes
Validity
Concurrent validity (convergent, divergent):
Zampini L (2009) - Italian CDI
- Correlation coefficient used: Pearson's correlation coefficient
- Measure: Brunet-Lézine score
- Results: Significant correlation was found between the Brunet-Lézine score and the children's vocabulary production assessed using the Italien CDI PVB
- Population/Disease: Patients with ; n= Patients with Down Syndrome ; n= 20; age= 36-month old (range: 35.28-38.18)
Known-group validity:
Trauner DA (2013)
Known-groups validity:
1. Measure/Groups of patients: Children who had peri-natal stroke in the Right Hemisphere (RH) or Left Hemisphere (LH) versus control: LH (n= 28), RH (n= 14), Control (n= 85)
- A priori hypotheses: Not stated
- Were hypotheses confirmed: Not applicable
- Results: ANOVA and Post Hoc test
Significant differences were found between groups:
Later gestures subscale: F= 6.52, p=0.002
Total gestures: F= 4.55, p= 0.012
Later gestures subscale:
RH vs Control: p= 0.0001
LH vs RH: p= 0.019
Total gestures:
RH versus Control: p= 0.011
LH versus RH: p= 0.030
- Population/Disease: Patients with peri-natal stroke; n= 127, aged 12 months
Reznick (2001)
2- Measure/Groups of patients: MacArthur-Bates CDI Words and Gestures scores assessed at different children's age: 8 months, 10 months, 12 months.
- A priori hypotheses: Not stated
- Were hypotheses confirmed: Not applicable
- Results: ANOVA, p<0.01 for all results
ANOVA revealed significant differences in MacArthur-Bates CDI: Words and Gestures scores at different children's ages:
8 vs 10 months: F(1.127)= 14.32
10 vs 12 months: F(1.127)= 55.51
- Population/Disease: Children from 8 to 12 months; n= 39
3- Measure/Groups of patients: MacArthur-Bates CDI Words and Gestures scores assessed at different children's ages: 10 months, 12 months.
- A priori hypotheses: Not stated
- Were hypotheses confirmed: Not applicable
- Results: ANOVA, p<0.01 for all results
ANOVA revealed significant difference in MacArthur-Bates CDI: Words and Gestures scores at different children's ages:
10 vs 12 months: F(1.127)= 42.72
- Population/Disease: Children from 10 to 12 months; n= 20
4- Measure/Groups of patients: MacArthur-Bates CDI Words and Gestures comprehension scale scores assessed at different children's ages: 8 months, 10 months, 12 months.
- A priori hypotheses: Not stated
- Were hypotheses confirmed: Not applicable
- Results: ANOVA, p<0.01 for all results
ANOVA revealed significant differences in MacArthur-Bates CDI: Words and Gestures comprehension scale scores at different children's ages:
8-10 months: F(1.36)= 44.49
10-12 months: F(1.36)= 56.56
- Population/Disease: Children from 8 to 12 months; n= 39
5- Measure/Groups of patients: MacArthur-Bates CDI Words and Gestures comprehension scale scores assessed at different children's ages: 10 months, 12 months.
- A priori hypotheses: Not stated
- Were hypotheses confirmed: Not applicable
- Results: ANOVA, p<0.01 for all results
ANOVA revealed significant difference in MacArthur-Bates CDI: Words and Gestures comprehension scale scores at different children's ages:
10 vs 12 months: F(1.19)= 28.29
- Population/Disease: Children from 10 to 12 months; n= 20
6- Measure/Groups of patients: MacArthur-Bates CDI Words and Gestures comprehension scale scores across 3 cohorts. Each cohort represents a different child's age: 8 months (n=39), 10 months (n=20), and 12 months (n=20)
- A priori hypotheses: Not stated
- Were hypotheses confirmed: Not applicable
- Results: ANOVA, p<0.01
ANOVA revealed a significant difference in MacArthur-Bates CDI: Words and Gestures scores at different children's ages: F(2.72)= 17.82
- Population/Disease: Children between 8 and 12 months; n= 79
Fenson (1994)
7. Measure/Groups of patients: Correlation between the MacArthur-Bates CDI Words and Gestures and age
- A priori hypotheses: Not stated
- Were hypotheses confirmed: Not applicable
- Results: Pearson's correlation coefficient and ANOVA, p<0.001 for all results
Pearson's correlations between age and:
Phrase comprehension: r= 0.63
Word comprehension: r=0.60
Word production: r=0.47
Actions and gestures: r=0.74
ANOVA:
Word comprehension was significantly affected by age, F(8.642)= 47.56
Word production was significantly affected by age, F(8.641)= 29.56
Actions and gestures were significantly affected by age, F(8.641)= 104.63
- Population/Disease: Children between 8 and 16 months; n= 659
Zampini L (2009) - Italian CDI
1. Measure/Groups of patients: Correlation between the number of words uttered during an observation session (20 min) and the Italian CDI Il Primo Vocabolario del Bambino (PVB))
- A priori hypotheses: Not stated
- Were hypotheses confirmed: Not applicable
- Results: Pearson's correlation coefficient
Significant correlation was found between the number of words and the PVB: r= 0.73; p<0.0001)
- Population/Disease: Patients with Down Syndrome ; n= 20; age= 36-month old (range: 35.28-38.18)
2. Measure/Groups of patients: Correlation between gesture token (deictic, conventional or iconic gesture) during an observation session (20 min) and the Italian CDI Il Primo Vocabolario del Bambino (PVB)) Comprehension
- A priori hypotheses: Not stated
- Were hypotheses confirmed: Not applicable
- Results: Pearson's correlation coefficient
Significant correlation was found between deictic gesture and the PVB: r= 0.21; p<0.05)
Significant correlation was found between conventional gesture and the PVB: r= 0.50; p<0.05)
Significant correlation was found between iconic gesture and the PVB: r= 0.45; p<0.05)
- Population/Disease: Patients with Down Syndrome ; n= 20; age= 36-month old (range: 35.28-38.18)
Evidence of Translatability Assessment: None identified
Evidence related to missing data: None identified
Evidence for Selection of Recall Period: None identified
Evidence of Administration Instructions and Training Provided: Yes
Evidence of concurrent validity: Yes
Evidence of known-groups validity: Yes
Evidence of ability to detect change over time: None identified
Ability to Detect Change
Ability to detect change (Responsiveness):
None identified
Responder Thresholds
Responder Thresholds:
None identified
Evidence of responder thresholds: None identified
Reference(s) of development / validation
Fenson L, Dale PS, Reznick JS, Bates E, Thai D. & Pethick S. Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1994; 59 (5, Serial No. 242) (Full text article: https://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/1166093.pdf)
Fenson L, Dale PS, Reznick JS, Thai D, Bates E, Hartung JP, Pethick S, & Reilly JS. The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User's guide and technical manual. San Diego: Singular Publishing Group. 1993
Fenson L, Resznick S, Thal D, Bates E, Hartung J, Reilly J. The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. 1992
Other references
Molinaro M, Broman AT, Rathouz PJ, Hustad KC. Longitudinal Development of Receptive Vocabulary in Children with Cerebral Palsy and Anarthria: Use of the MacArthur-Bates CDI. Dev Neurorehabil. 2020 Jul;23(5):285-293 (Full text article: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/pmid/31352864/)
Heilmann J, Ellis Weismer S, Evans J, Hollar C. Utility of the MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventory in identifying language abilities of late-talking and typically developing toddlers. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2005 Feb;14(1):40-51 (PubMed abstract: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15966111/)
Feldman HM, Dollaghan CA, Campbell TF, Kurs-Lasky M, Janosky JE, Paradise JL. Measurement properties of the MacArthur communicative development inventories at ages one and two years. Child Dev. 2000 Mar-Apr;71(2):310-22 (Full text article: https://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/1131991.pdf)
Arriaga RI, Fenson L, Cronan T, & Pethick SJ. Scores on the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory of Children From Low and Middle Income Levels. Applied Psycholinguistics. 1988. 19, 209-223 (abstract: https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/applied-psycholinguistics/article/abs/scores-on-the-macarthur-communicative-development-inventory-of-children-from-lowand-middleincome-families/C5AB9E2B53A6D64178FC5F0D7FCCB76C)
See (PubMed results: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/?term=%22MBCDI%22&sort=date&sort_order=asc)
Inclusion of the COA in product labelling
None identified
Existence of Scoring / Interpretation / User Manual
Original language and translations
Original: English for the USA
Translations:
Sindhi
Sasak
Samoan
Ronga
Northern Sami
Ngasa
Mandinka
Yao
Faroese for the Faroe Islands
Yiddish
Xhosa
Wolof
Vietnamese
Urdu
Turkish
Tongan
Thai
Tamil
Tagalog
Swedish
Spanish for Peru
Spanish for Mexico
Spanish for Spain
Spanish for Cuba
Spanish for Columbia
Spanish for Chile
Spanish for Argentina
Southern Sotho
Slovenian
Slovak
Shona
Setswana
Serbian
Russian
Portuguese for Mozambique
Portuguese for Portugal
Portuguese for Brazil
Polish
Persian
Norwegian
Maltese
Mandarin for Singapore
Mandarin for Taiwan
Mandarin for China
Mandarin for Malaysia
Malayalam
Malay for Malaysia
Latvian
Korean
Kriol for Sierra Leone
Kinyarwanda
Kannada
Japanese
italian
Inuktitut
Icelandic
Hungarian
Hindi
Hebrew
Greenlandic for Greenland
Greek
German for Switzerland
Greek
German for Switzerland
German for Austria
German
Galician
French for France
French for Canada
Finnish
Estonian
English for South Africa
English for Singapore
English for South Africa
English for New Zealand
English for Australia
English for the UK
Dutch
Danish
Czech
Croatian
Chichewa
Cantonese for Hong Kong
Catalan
Bulgarian
Basque
Bengali
Arabic for Tunisia
Arabic for Saudi Arabia
Arabic for Palestine
Arabic
Arabic for Kuwait
Albanian
Afrikaans
References of translations
Arabic, English and French for Lebanon
Marianne Daccache, Camille Messarra, Christophe dos Santos. Les premiers mots de l’enfant libanais en contexte plurilingue : que nous apprend l’utilisation d’un compte rendu parental trilingue ?. Lexique, Presses Universitaires du Septentrion, 2020, 26, pp.7-28 (Full text article: https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02898336/document)
English for Ireland
O’Toole, C. (2013). Using parent report to assess bilingual vocabulary acquisition: A model from Irish. In Mueller Gathercole, V. C. (Eds.), Solutions for the assessment of bilinguals (pp. 81–102). Bristol: Multilingual Matters (Abstract: https://cora.ucc.ie/handle/10468/2629)
Faroese for the Faroe Islands
Rasmussen SM, Bleses D. Faroese children’s early vocabulary acquisition: A Faroese adaptation of the MacArthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventories. University of Southern Denmark 38 (6): 641-668
French for France
Kern S. Le compte-rendu parental au service de l'évaluation de la production lexicale des enfants français entre 16 et 30 mois. Glossa, 85, 48-62. 2003 (Full text article: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/283919229_Le_compte-rendu_parental_au_service_de_l'evaluation_de_la_production_lexicale_des_enfants_francais_entre_16_et_30_mois)
Schmengler H, El-Khoury Lesueur F, Yermachenko A, Taine M, Cohen D, Peyre H, Saint-Georges C, Thierry X, Melchior M. Maternal immigrant status and signs of neurodevelopmental problems in early childhood: The French representative ELFE birth cohort. Autism Res. 2019 Dec;12(12):1845-1859 (PubMed abstract: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/31373761/)
French for Canada
Trudeau N et al. Les Inventaires MacArthur-Bates du développement de la communication (IMBCD): Manuel technique et guide de l’utilisateur. Candadian Language and Literacy Research Network. 2008 (Full text article: https://eoa.umontreal.ca/wp-content/uploads/sites/32/docMacArthurBates_Manuel_MacArthur.pdf)
Hebrew for Israel
Gendler-Shalev H, Dromi E. The Hebrew Web Communicative Development Inventory (MB-CDI): Lexical Development Growth Curves. J Child Lang. 2021 Mar 19:1-17 (Full text article: https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/journal-of-child-language/article/hebrew-web-communicative-development-inventory-mbcdi-lexical-development-growth-curves/80A8AFEEB4ED71E3682560A77FBD7941)
Gendler-Shalev H. The adaptation of HCDI-WG parent questionnaire to Hebrew. Unpublished MA thesis. Tel-Aviv University. 2005. [In Hebrew]
Copy of the Spanish translation can be obtained here: https://mb-cdi.stanford.edu/forms.html
The MB-CDI is available in the following sign language:
American sign language
British sign language
Israeli sign language
Italian sign language
Spanish sign language
Turkish sign language
The MB-CDI is available in the following dialects:
Aboriginal English
Changana
English for Wales
Jamaican Creole
Kishwahili/kigiriama
Klingon
Malawian
North Australian Kriol
Romani
For more information on adaptations and translations of the MB-CDI see here: https://mb-cdi.stanford.edu/adaptations.html
Condition of use: copyright
Copyright:
CDI Advisory Board holds the copyright
CoU:
*With fees for academic/non profit research
*With fees for commercial/pharmaceutical companies
*The manual as well as the forms can be ordered from Brookes Publishing. For further information please contact directly Larry Fenson
Website
MacArthur Bates CDI website: https://mb-cdi.stanford.edu/about.html
Review copy
MB-CDI-WG available in Fenson et al. 1994
https://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/1166093.pdf